|
Close
window to return
|
Issue 9, September
2001
Psycho-social stress, lifestyle and socio-economic
inequalities in morbidity and mortality
George Davey Smith, John Macleod, Carole Hart, Pauline Heslop and
Chris Metcalfe
- The determinants
of health inequalities are only partially understood. It has been suggested
that psycho-social factors, such as perceived stress, are important
in this regard. This suggestion currently enjoys widespread support,
however, the evidence, which comes mainly from observational studies,
should be interpreted with caution.
- Many studies are
cross-sectional, making the direction of causality between stress and
disease impossible to determine. Most measurements of stress exposures
and many health outcomes include a significant subjective component.
Both may be influenced by a common reporting tendency generating apparent,
though spurious, associations between stress and health. Similarly,
an association between stress and social disadvantage will automatically
lead to an apparent association between stress and health due to confounding
by other correlates of deprivation.
- We examined the
relationships between perceived stress, social position, lifestyle,
physiological risk factors and both morbidity and mortality within a
workplace derived cohort of around 6,000 men and 1,200 women recruited
in Scotland in the early 1970s and followed up for over 20 years. Half
this cohort was re-examined 5 years after initial recruitment. Angina
and ischaemia were among the health outcomes we examined.
- Higher stress was
associated with social advantage in men, but not women. Cross-sectionally,
greater stress was associated with unhealthy behaviour (greater smoking,
greater alcohol consumption and less exercise) but with a mixed profile
of physiological risk factors. The pattern was broadly similar for both
men and women, although high stress was associated with lower body mass
index in men and with higher body mass index in women. In men, stress
showed a strong cross-sectional association with self-reported angina
but not with ischaemia on ECG. In men, higher stress was associated
with a greater than doubling of the risk of incident angina. However,
higher stress was also associated with an apparent reduction in the
risk of incident ECG ischaemia.
- Amongst men, higher
stress was associated with an apparently reduced risk of both all cause
mortality and mortality from coronary heart disease. A similar relationship
with mortality was seen for cumulative stress at first and second screening
and for stress that increased between first and second screening. After
adjustment for social position, all of these relationships were attenuated.
Amongst women, there was little difference in the risk of all cause
mortality according to perceived stress. Only alcohol related mortality
and mortality from respiratory diseases in men showed the association
with stress predicted by the relationship between stress and behaviour.
However, the suggestion of increase in both these indices with higher
stress was weak and of small magnitude. This suggested a primacy of
material circumstances in determining health.
- The number of male
hospital admissions for cardiovascular disease, alcohol-related illness
and psychiatric illness increased with increasing stress. There was
no convincing evidence of an effect of stress on female hospitalisation,
although a similar trend to that of men was found.
- Our findings illustrate
some of the pitfalls around the interpretation of observational evidence
in this area. Amongst men, we found an apparently increased risk of
angina and admission to hospital with higher stress. This contrasted
with an apparently decreased risk of ischaemic change on ECG and of
mortality. We suggest this anomaly arose due to the influence of bias
in relation to the former outcomes and confounding in relation to the
latter. In the absence of evidence from controlled trials, this suggests
that interventions targeting psycho-social factors are not likely to
be an effective strategy to reduce health inequalities. A focus on the
reduction of material inequalities alongside interventions aimed at
established risk factors seems more appropriate.
Background
The policy
aim of reducing social inequalities in health is complicated by an incomplete
understanding of the processes that generate these inequalities. Psycho-social
factors, such as perceived stress, are plausible causes related to health
through direct neuro-endocrine pathways or through their promotion of
unhealthy behaviour. Since these factors are often related to social position
it has been suggested that they may be important determinants of social
inequalities in health, particularly inequalities in coronary heart disease.
It has been further suggested that interventions targeting these factors
may be an effective strategy to reduce health inequalities.
The importance of
psycho-social factors, particularly stress, to health enjoys widespread
popular credence. However, scientific evidence supporting a causal role
for these exposures as important determinants of morbidity and mortality
derives almost exclusively from observational studies. Several considerations
suggest that such evidence should be interpreted with caution.
First, many studies
are cross-sectional, making the direction of causality between exposure
and outcome impossible to determine. Causation may run mainly from health
to stress, rather than vice versa. For example, it is likely that the
experience of ill health will increase perception - and hence reporting
- of stress. Second, where the measurement of both exposure and outcome
depend substantially on individual subjective perception (both of 'stress'
and of illness), both may be inflated by a common reporting tendency,
producing spurious associations. Third, rather than 'explaining' social
inequalities in health, the fact that health and psycho-social factors
like stress are both related to social position could generate an apparent,
but non-causal, association between the two. Confounding of this nature
may lead to associations that remain even after adjustment for social
position is made in the statistical model. Apparently robust associations
would remain if the psycho-social factor captures a dimension of social
position which is not adequately measured by the indicator of social position
being used in the analysis. Finally, effects of stress on health may be
best understood through consideration of cumulative or changing stress
over time and over extended periods of follow-up.
Data and methods
The West
of Scotland Collaborative Study involves around 6,000 men and 1,200 women
recruited from 27 workplaces in 1971-73. Half this cohort was re-screened
in 1977. Screening included measurement of perceived stress (via the Reeder
Stress Inventory), physiological risk factors, behavioural risk factors,
indicators of social position and indices of coronary morbidity (Rose
angina questionnaire and ECG coded according to the Minnesota system).
Cause specific mortality data (ICD-9) are available for 21 years of follow-up.
Morbidity data from the Scottish Cancer Registry and the Scottish Morbidity
Register (hospital admissions) were included from the same period.
Stress at first screening
was divided into three categories (high, medium, low). For participants
screened twice, stress was divided into the same categories of cumulative
stress score and into increasing, stable and decreasing perceived stress.
A score variable was derived based on a count of complaints of somatic
symptoms showing no relationship to mortality over 21 years of follow-up.
Reporting of these symptoms was assumed to relate primarily to reporting
tendency rather than underlying somatic pathology.
Cross-sectional relationships
between stress, social position, lifestyle, risk factors, reporting tendency
and morbidity were examined. Incidence of new coronary heart disease (angina
or ischaemia) at second screening by stress category at first screening
was calculated in logistic regression models. All cause and cause specific
mortality by stress category at first screening, cumulative stress at
first and second screening and change in stress over the screening interval
was calculated in proportional hazards models.
Findings
Perceived
stress showed a graded association with occupational class in men, from
highest mean stress in Social Class I to lowest in Social Class V. No
association between stress and occupational class was apparent amongst
women. Before adjustment for social position, higher stress was associated
with an adverse profile of behavioural risk (more smoking and greater
alcohol consumption in men and women, less exercise in men only) but a
mixed profile of physiological risk (lower blood pressure and higher cholesterol
in men and women, lower BMI in men, higher BMI in women). Adjustment for
occupational class strengthened the association with unhealthy behaviour.
Apart from BMI, none of the physiological risk factors were associated
with stress following this adjustment.
Reported stress showed
a strong, direct association with reporting tendency in men. Prevalent
angina at first screening significantly increased with both increasing
perceived stress and increasing reporting tendency score. Table 1 shows
the odds for incident angina and incident ischaemia, comparing those with
medium and high stress to those with low stress. The odds for incident
angina were more than doubled amongst high, compared to low stress men.
By contrast, the odds for incident ischaemia were almost halved in high,
compared to low stress men (Table 1). Incident angina was also significantly
increased amongst those participants with high reporting tendency scores.
Adding reporting tendency score to the model attenuated the above effect
estimates but in the case of angina they remained very strong.
Table 2 shows the
relative risk of mortality, both for all causes of mortality and for specific
causes of death, again comparing men with high and medium stress to those
with low stress. It shows that the relative hazard of all cause mortality
was reduced in medium and high, compared to low stress participants. These
reductions were small and were attenuated following adjustment for current
occupational class. Most classes of cause specific mortality followed
this pattern. Cumulative stress scores showed a similar pattern with mortality
as did change in stress score (i.e. reduced hazard of mortality amongst
participants with increasing or stable stress scores compared to decreasing
stress scores). Amongst women, there was little difference in the relative
hazard of all-cause mortality or cause-specific mortality according to
perceived stress, though this analysis was limited due to smaller sample
size.
Following adjustment
for age and socio-economic status, the number of hospital admissions for
cardiovascular disease, alcohol-related illness and psychiatric illness
in men increased with increasing stress. Deaths per admission showed a
trend in the opposite direction in the case of cardiovascular disease.
No convincing evidence of an effect of stress on hospitalisation was found
for women.
Future research
These findings
illustrate the pitfalls of observational research on psycho-social exposures
and in particular, the influence of bias and confounding. Future research
must address these issues. The use of objective exposure and outcome measures
reduces the potential for bias. Studying psycho-social factors in populations
where they are not proxies for social disadvantage is a partial solution
to the problem of confounding. Experimental studies are needed to address
this issue fully, and to determine whether psycho-social interventions
are a promising strategy to improve population health.
Policy implications
It has been
suggested that psycho-social factors may hold the key to reductions in
health inequalities; these data cast doubt on this suggestion. Previous
studies that have shown an association between stress and objectively
poorer health have been undertaken in populations where stress was also
related to social disadvantage. It is likely that apparent relationships
with health were the product of confounding. In the present study such
confounding produced an association between stress and better health because
stress was related to social advantage, particularly in men. This was
despite an association between stress and unhealthy behaviour.
These findings have
important implications for policy. First, though individual behaviours
undoubtedly contribute to health, the contribution of material circumstances
is more important. Strategies to reduce health inequalities should reflect
this. Second, interventions to reduce psychological stress and improve
the psycho-social environment are humanitarian imperatives, arguably in
need of no further epidemiological justification. However, unless improvements
in the psycho-social environment, are accompanied by improvements in the
material environment they are unlikely to lead to better health.
Table1: Stress
and incident angina and ischaemia (men only)
Table 2: Relative
risk of mortality (95%CI) associated with perceived stress at first screening
(men only)
This project was funded by the ESRC Health Variations Programme from August
1997 to July 2000.
For further information,
please contact:
Professor George Davey
Smith
Department of Social Medicine
Canynge Hall
Whiteladies Road
Bristol
BS8 2PR
Email: George.Davey-Smith@Bristol.ac.uk
Telephone: 0117-9287201
Selected publications:
Heslop, P., Davey
Smith, G., Carroll, D., Macleod, J., Hyland, F. and Hart, C. (2001) 'Perceived
stress and coronary heart disease risk-factors: the contribution of socio-economic
position' British Journal of Health Psychology 6: 167-178.
Metcalfe, C., Davey Smith, G., Sterne, J. A. C., Heslop, P., Macleod,
J. and Hart, C. (2001) 'Individual employment histories and subsequent
cause specific hospital admissions and mortality: a prospective study
of a cohort of male and female workers with 21 years follow up' Journal
of Epidemiology and Community Health 55: 503-504.
Heslop, P., Davey Smith, G., Macleod, J. and Hart, C. (in press 2001)
'The socioeconomic position of employed women, risk factors and mortality'
Social Science and Medicine.
Heslop, P., Davey Smith, G., Macleod, J., Metcalfe, C. and Hart, C. (in
press 2001) 'Job satisfaction, self-reported stress, cardiovascular risk
factors and mortality' Social Science and Medicine.
Heslop, P., Davey Smith, G., Macleod, J., Metcalfe, C., Carroll, D. and
Hart, C. (in press 2001) 'Are the effects of psychosocial exposures attributable
to confounding? Evidence from a prospective observational study on psychological
stress and mortality' Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health.
|